We also generated a precise excision of the ppk11Mi transposon th

We also generated a precise excision of the ppk11Mi transposon that restores the ppk11 gene locus, assessed by sequence analysis. After the addition PD0325901 cell line of PhTx to the precise excision background (ppk11Precise),

the EPSP amplitude is returned to the size it was in the absence of PhTx (ppk11Precise; Figure 1D), and the homeostatic enhancement of presynaptic neurotransmitter release is restored to wild-type levels ( Figure 1E). Taken together, these data support the conclusion that disruption of the ppk11 gene blocks the rapid induction of synaptic homeostasis. Despite observing a complete block of synaptic homeostasis, there is no consistent alteration in baseline synaptic transmission caused by the loss of ppk11. First, we compare wild-type Screening Library chemical structure ( Figure 2B, black bars) with the ppk11PBac mutation ( Figure 2B, dark blue bars) and find no significant change in presynaptic release and only a minor change in mEPSP amplitude. Second, we compare the ppk11Mi mutation ( Figure 2B, light blue bars) with its appropriate genetic control, the precise excision of the ppk11Mi transposon (ppk11Precise; Figure 2B, open bars). There is no change in baseline release when this comparison is made. We note that there is a significant (p < 0.01) difference

in baseline release when we compare wild-type with either ppk11Mi or the ppk11Precise control line, and we attribute this to differences in genetic background. Third, we analyzed a trans-heterozygous combination of independently derived ppk11 mutations

(ppk11Mi/ ppk11PBac) and find no change in quantal content compared to wild-type ( Figure S1). In this trans-heterozygous combination, there is a decrease in mEPSP amplitude that correlates with a decrease in postsynaptic muscle input resistance (WT = 8.1 MΩ compared to ppk11Mi/ ppk11PBac = 3.9 MΩ; p < 0.01). Terminal deoxynucleotidyl transferase From these data, we conclude that disruption of ppk11 blocks synaptic homeostasis without altering baseline release, specifically when mutations are compared to their appropriate genetic control. This conclusion is supported by several additional experiments, presented below. We next examined synaptic homeostasis and baseline transmission at elevated external calcium (1 mM) that is within the range of what is thought to be physiological calcium (Figures 2C–2E). We first quantified mEPSP amplitudes in current-clamp mode, in which the signal-to-noise ratio is excellent, and then switched to two-electrode voltage-clamp mode to measure evoked synaptic currents. We observe a decrease in mEPSP amplitude when PhTx is applied to the wild-type NMJ at 1 mM calcium and we find that EPSC amplitudes are unchanged in the presence of PhTx, as expected for precise homeostatic compensation.

75 and 43 5 mg/ml ( Macedo et al , 2009) Ocimum gratissimum esse

75 and 43.5 mg/ml ( Macedo et al., 2009). Ocimum gratissimum essential oil inhibited 100% egg hatching at concentration of 0.5% ( Pessoa et al., 2002). Croton zehntneri and Lippia sidoides essential oils inhibited egg hatching in more than 98% at 1.25 mg/ml and larval development in over 98% INCB024360 cell line at 10 mg/ml ( Camurça-Vasconcelos et al., 2007). Chenopodium ambrosioides essential oil inhibited 100% egg hatching at 1.33 μl/ml ( Ketzis et al., 2002). The LC50 of Eucalyptus staigeriana essential oil in the EHA was 0.324 mg/ml and LC50 in the LDA was 1.702 mg/ml ( Macedo et al., 2010). Those values are higher in comparison to our results that showed LC50 in EHA of 0.04; 0.26 and 0.13 mg/ml and LC50 in LDA of 0.06; 0.26 and 0.15 mg/ml

to C. schoenanthus, M. piperita and C. martinii essential oils, respectively. Terpenes are a chemical class of chemicals found in essential oils. C. schoenanthus had approximately 20 constituents, being rich in geraniol, geranial, and neral. Terpenoid compounds are known to be active against a range of organisms and the synergy of several terpenoids can be effective on several targets because they are a complex mixture of compounds that can interact with multiple molecular targets on various developmental stages of the parasite ( Marie-Magdeleine et al., 2009). So, it is quite reasonable to Apoptosis Compound Library nmr consider that the major constituents of

each plant species, as detected by gas chromatography, had some biological activity in vitro against trichostrongylids in the present study. Because geraniol was the main component in both Cymbopogon species, all which had better anthelmintic effects than Mentha (devoid of geraniol), we can hypothesize that geraniol might be of potential interest for in vivo tests. However, the concentration of geraniol does not preclude the potential synergistic effect of geranial and neral, present at higher concentrations in C. schoenanthus, the essential oil with the best anthelmintic activity. The insolubility of essential oils and many of their constituents in aqueous media is likely to impair their performance in susceptibility tests,

and attempts to overcome this problem have been made by using tensio-active agents such as Tween 20 and Tween 80 (Juven et al., 1994). Although Tween 80 has low toxicity to nematodes (4.79% inhibition hatchability and 3.23% inhibition exsheathment) compared to Tween 20, it caused toxicity on LFIA and LDA assays. DMSO, which is less toxic to L1 larvae than Tween 80, was used for both LFIA (14.53% inhibition feeding activity) and LDA (12.35% inhibition development). Those solvents can work as bioenhancers, as they have the ability to increase the bioavailability of drugs by increasing their transport across membranes, increasing anthelmintic effect. Thus, the type and concentration of solvents required to make an emulsion should be considered before tests are performed. Depending on the organism, the solvents can be highly lethal.

Between blocks, lengthening or returning the delay

to its

Between blocks, lengthening or returning the delay

to its standard length brought about robust changes in temporal firing patterns, even though the rats occupied the same selleck locations at comparable times in all trial blocks. These results show that retiming is not attributable to differences in behavior during delays of different lengths but, rather, is caused by altering a highly salient temporal parameter that characterizes the delay event. Combining these findings, changing the duration of the delay revealed that, while a minority of neurons encode absolute or relative time, the majority form qualitatively distinct representations when the critical temporal cue was altered, and most of these maintain the new patterns when the delay is shortened to the original length. In order to assess whether a neuronal ensemble tracked the passage of time at each trial period, we used a two-way ANOVA using factors lag and trial period

to compare the similarity of the population vector at different lags during the object, odor, and first 1.2 s (early) and last 1.2 s (late) phases of the delay period. This analysis revealed a main effect of lag (F(4, 20) = 34.74; p < 0.001), trial period (F(3,15) = 9.94; p = 0.001), and an interaction between the two factors (F(12,60) = 3.17; p = 0.002). Separate one-way ANOVAs confirmed a main effect of lag (all p values <0.002) and a significant linear component (all p values <0.03) selleck kinase inhibitor such that the population vector became less similar as lag increased during all trial periods, indicating temporal coding throughout the trial. Furthermore, a comparison of the change in the similarity of the population vector between lag 1 and lag Sodium butyrate 5 (ΔL) indicates that time is coded at higher resolution early in the

trial (F(1, 11) = 23.81, p < 0.001; ΔL for delay early and object compared to ΔL for delay late and odor in Figure 4B). We also conducted GLM analyses to directly compare the extent to which time and location influence firing during the object and odor periods; these analyses do not consider other behavioral variables. Unlike the delay neurons, the activity from almost three-quarters (72/99 or 72%) of the neurons active in the object period was best explained by space or time, but not both variables. For 43 (60%) of these 72 object neurons, the inclusion of space without time in the model provided a more parsimonious account of the data. In 29 neurons (40%), time by itself was sufficient to explain neural activity, and the proportion of these neurons was different than that explained by space (χ21 = 4.70; p = 0.03). For the remaining 27 out of 99 object neurons, activity was explained best by both time and space, and the STIC from 13 of these neurons favored time while that of 14 neurons favored space. The results obtained from neurons active during the odor period were similar.

This modulation of eCB-LTD by D2 and A2A receptors requires RGS4,

This modulation of eCB-LTD by D2 and A2A receptors requires RGS4, which is phosphorylated by PKA (Huang et al., 2007) and inhibits mGluR-Gq signaling (Saugstad et al., 1998). RGS4 is therefore a key link between dopamine signaling, synaptic plasticity, and motor behavior, and may be a promising non-dopaminergic target for modulating basal ganglia circuitry. Our finding of two distinct forms of eCB-LTD (HFS-LTD and LFS-LTD) in the same cell type both clarifies previous findings and raises new questions. Our data agree with previous studies indicating Ceritinib molecular weight that AEA is the eCB

mediating HFS-LTD while highlighting the importance of PLD for its production (Ade and Lovinger, 2007 and Kreitzer and Malenka, 2007). However, other studies of striatal eCB-LTD have indicated that 2-AG is the eCB that mediates striatal eCB-LTD (Fino et al., 2010 and Lerner et al., 2010). Indeed, we confirm that 2-AG can also mediate eCB-LTD, given KRX-0401 supplier the right stimulation frequency and duration, thus helping to

resolve some of the apparent conflicts in the literature. Additionally, our LFS-LTD data fit well with a previous study of striatal LTD using low-frequency stimulation (Ronesi and Lovinger, 2005), which until now was difficult to reconcile with studies of HFS-LTD. Like this LTD, which was induced by 5 min of continuous 10 Hz stimulation, our LFS-LTD is prevented by blockers of CB1 receptors and D2 receptors, but not by L-VGCC blockers or by calcium chelation with BAPTA. Together with our data, these findings demonstrate that eCB-LTD can be calcium-independent, most likely because PLCβ can be sufficiently activated by prolonged group I mGluR activation alone. Both HFS-LTD and LFS-LTD are

modulated by dopamine D2 receptors and adenosine A2A receptors and this modulation of LTD appears to be important for regulating motor function (Kreitzer and Malenka, 2007 and Lerner et al., 2010). Here Phosphatidylinositol diacylglycerol-lyase we provide the first evidence of a specific mechanism by which D2 and A2A receptor modulation of LTD occurs, via cAMP/PKA mediated regulation of RGS4 activity. While our experiments argue that D2 and A2A receptors regulate LTD induction mainly via their downstream signaling pathways, we cannot rule out a role for physical interactions of D2 and A2A receptors in the membrane, which have been reported (for review see Fuxe et al., 2005 and Fuxe et al., 2007), though not studied in the context of LTD. D2 and A2A receptors appear to regulate cAMP accumulation in MSNs primarily by acting on adenylyl cyclase 5 (AC5), a striatal-enriched form of adenylyl cyclase, since mice lacking AC5 have impaired striatal synaptic plasticity.

These results are consistent with miniature neurotransmission als

These results are consistent with miniature neurotransmission also being necessary for synaptic ultrastructural maturation in addition to morphological expansion. We next sought to establish the nature of the developmental signal induced by miniature neurotransmission. Synaptic structure can be influenced by factors that act locally at the individual terminal level (e.g., synaptic adhesion factors; Davis

and Goodman, 1998) or throughout the whole neuron (e.g., transcriptional regulation; McCabe et al., 2003). Action potentials and evoked NT affect the entire synaptic terminal. In contrast, we surmised that the effects of miniature NT might be spatially see more restricted to individual active zones and therefore could act locally to regulate bouton maturation. To test this hypothesis, we examined the synaptic terminals generated by the type Ib motor neuron RP3 (Hoang and Chiba, 2001). The single axon of this neuron bifurcates to produce synaptic terminals concurrently on two postsynaptic targets: muscle 6 and muscle 7 (Figure 7A). The ratio of synaptic boutons produced at each muscle is stereotyped (Davis and Goodman, 1998),

and these muscles are not electrically coupled with each other (Ueda and Kidokoro, 1996), facilitating independent manipulation of NT. We used Gal4 drivers expressed either in muscle 6 alone (but not muscle 7) (Figures 7A and S7A) or in both muscles to dissect if miniature NT signaling acts locally at terminals or throughout the neuron. Similar to other synapses, reduction of miniature Thiazovivin manufacturer TCL NT by iGluRMUT reduced typical bouton numbers ( Figures 7B, 7C, and 7F) and increased the fraction of small boutons ( Figure 7G) at both of the RP3 MN terminals on muscles 6 and 7 compared to controls, though the area of these terminals could not be accurately measured due their complex spatial arrangement.

When we overexpressed a wild-type iGluR transgene (UAS- dGluRWT) in both postsynaptic muscles of iGluRMUT mutants, we restored normal miniature NT at both terminals ( Figure S7B). This also fully rescued bouton numbers and the bouton size index at both terminals ( Figures 7D, 7F, and 7G). We next expressed UAS-dGluRWT only in muscle 6 of iGluRMUT mutants. This increased miniature NT at muscle 6 terminals without altering NT at muscle 7 ( Figure S7B). When we examined the morphology of both terminals, we found that bouton numbers and bouton size were restored at terminals at muscle 6 ( Figures 7E–7G). In contrast, however, the terminals at muscle 7 were not rescued ( Figures 7E–7G). Because both terminals are produced by a single neuron, this result suggested that the effect of reduced miniature NT on synaptic bouton maturation is localized to individual terminals. In a complementary experiment, we examined the suppression of cpx mutants using a similar strategy.

In our LPFC recordings, the frequency of recording from such neur

In our LPFC recordings, the frequency of recording from such neurons appeared to be much higher (46% of the total number of modulated cells during the BFS condition; n = 54/118) than the respective percentage observed in cortical areas lower in the visual hierarchy, like V1 (10%, n = 10/104; Keliris et al., 2010), V4 (30%, n = 8/26; Leopold and Logothetis,

1996), and MT (26%, n = 12/46; Logothetis and Schall, 1989). However, encountering such cells is most likely the result of weak and variable stimulus preferences. In our data, when interaction effects (Stimulus × Condition) were explicitly tested using an ANOVA, only 2% of cells (n = 10/577) were found to be significantly modulated LY2157299 (p < 0.05) only during BFS. We also studied whether local cortical processing reflected in the local population spiking activity within a prefrontal cortical site could represent subjective visual perception. When nonsorted multiunit spiking activity was examined (MUA, i.e., the sum of the spikes recorded from a tetrode before spike sorting), we BKM120 found further evidence that the spiking activity of neuronal populations in the LPFC follows reliably phenomenal perception. Our results show that 20% of the total number of recorded sites (n = 42/211) were significantly modulated during physical

alternation (Wilcoxon rank-sum test, p < 0.05). In the large majority of these sites, MUA was also found to be significantly modulated during BFS (n = 31/42, or 74%). During BFS, sensory preference was retained in 94% (n = 29/31) of these sites, and in only 6% of the sites (n = 2/31), neuronal discharges were found to reverse their preference and increased their firing rate when a preferred stimulus was perceptually suppressed (ANOVA, Mannose-binding protein-associated serine protease Stimulus × Condition interaction effect, p < 0.05). We compared the magnitude of sensory and perceptual modulation for the 42 MUA sites found to be significantly modulated

during physical alternation (Figure 2C). We found that MUA modulation during BFS was significantly decreased and reached 56% of the modulation observed during physical stimulus alternation (d′sensory MUA = 1.1 ± 0.14 and d′perceptual MUA = 0.62 ± 0.13, Wilcoxon rank-sum test p = 0.019). Both distributions were significantly different from zero (t test, p < 10−9 for d′sensory MUA, p < 10−6 for d′perceptual MUA), thus indicating that the level of mean perceptual modulation was also adequate to distinguish between preferred and nonpreferred stimuli during subjective perception. Similar to SUA, we found that in cases where MUA exhibited a particularly strong sensory modulation (d′sensory MUA > 1), the percentage of perceptually modulated recording sites was even higher. In such strongly modulated cases, 92% of the sensory selective recording sites (n = 12/13) were also significantly modulated during BFS (Figure 2D).

In that regard, the International Neuroimaging Data-sharing Initi

In that regard, the International Neuroimaging Data-sharing Initiative (INDI), a next-generation FCP endeavor, was founded in an attempt to (1) expand the scope of open data sharing in the functional neuroimaging community to include phenotypic data beyond age and sex (a limitation of the FCP data set) and (2) provide a model for prospective, prepublication data sharing. INDI has buy INCB018424 already demonstrated the feasibility of achieving these goals. In

particular, the Nathan Kline Institute-Rockland Sample (NKI-RS) successfully and prospectively distributed over 200 deeply phenotyped R-fMRI and diffusion tensor imaging data sets sampling the life span via weekly uploads. With recently granted National Institute of Mental Health funding, the NKI-RS effort will phenotype and image 1,000 individuals over the next 4 years—once again with weekly prepublication sharing, including R-fMRI and diffusion imaging data acquired via novel sequences (Feinberg et al., 2010) provided by the HCP. Numerous other prospective data LY2157299 solubility dmso sets have been pledged or provided, with varying distribution

schedules (e.g., quarterly). INDI also actively gathers and shares retrospective data sets. The Brain Genomics Superstruct Project, launched in 2008 at Harvard University and Massachusetts General Hospital, will share 1,500 data sets in 2012 (Yeo et al., 2011). The ADHD-200 was launched in March 2011, sharing data sets from 485 typically developing children and 291 children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder aggregated from eight independent imaging sites. This was a landmark

event for child psychiatry, for which data collection can be exceedingly costly and technically challenging. INDI offers a drastically less expensive means of accelerating science by providing large boluses of data upon which future efforts can be based. The FCP and INDI efforts are not unique in embracing open data sharing. The first, fMRIDC, successfully accumulated thousands of images and inspired researchers worldwide. Similarly, Brainscape, OASIS (http://www.oasis-brains.org) Cell press and XNAT Central (http://central.xnat.org) were established to encourage investigators to deposit data sets for open sharing. Additionally, the XNAT-based efforts aim to enable easy data accessioning and databasing: highly desirable goals that are increasingly being realized (Marcus et al., 2007). The mantle of T-fMRI data sharing is now being taken up again by Open fMRI (http://www.openfmri.org), which also provides analytic tools (Poldrack, 2011). Additionally, http://openconnectome.org has launched a data-sharing initiative for electron microscopy data that provides information about microlevel connectome properties.

1 mM sodium citrate, pH 6 0 at rt PCMCs without CaP and loaded s

1 mM sodium citrate, pH 6.0 at rt. PCMCs without CaP and loaded simultaneously with DT and CyaA* released DT almost instantaneously whilst the 6% and 20% CaP PCMCs displayed progressively delayed antigen release ( Fig. 1D). Similar results were obtained for all antigens and combinations tested, indicating that the phenomenon was not antigen-specific (not shown). BSA-FITC release from PCMCs suspended in PBS at 37 °C was investigated as a more physiologically relevant model. BSA-FITC release from PCMCs without CaP was extremely rapid but was significantly slower with CaP PCMCs ( Fig.

1E). Subcutaneous injection of mice with PCMCs loaded with DT in the absence of CaP induced significantly higher anti-DT IgG titres than the equivalent soluble antigen at both 28 d and 42 d (Fig. 2). Similar effects were seen with the other antigens indicating that this response was not antigen-specific (data not shown). Whilst selleck inhibitor Enzalutamide formulation into PCMCs

enhanced the immune response to DT, it was likely that surface modification with CaP would further enhance antigen-specific IgG titres. Mice were immunised with 0%, 6% or 20% CaP PCMCs loaded with DT, DT + CyaA* or BSA. CaP PCMCs enhanced the antigen-specific IgG response to DT and BSA at 28 d and 42 d post-immunisation (Fig. 3). For PCMCs loaded with DT alone, CaP modification increased serum anti-DT IgG titres prior to boosting (Fig. 3A) but the effect was more pronounced after boosting (Fig. 3B). Inclusion of CyaA* did not alter the adjuvant effect Thiamine-diphosphate kinase of CaP on the anti-DT IgG response at 28 d (Fig. 3C) and 42 d (Fig. 3D). The adjuvant activity of CaP was not confined to DT, as CaP PCMCs also promoted an increase in anti-BSA IgG titres at 28 d (Fig. 3E) and 42 d (Fig. 3F). Serum antigen-specific IgG1 and IgG2a titres were determined in order to assess whether CaP modification altered the Th1/Th2 bias. In mice, a decreased IgG1:IgG2a ratio is associated with a Th1-biased immune response [29]. Adsorption of DT to Al(OH)3 resulted in a high IgG1 response (Fig. 4A) and

a high anti-DT IgG1:IgG2a ratio (Fig. 4C) compared to soluble antigen or PCMC formulations. Increasing CaP loading increased both the anti-DT IgG1 and IgG2a titres (Fig. 4A and B) but the overall effect was to decrease the anti-DT IgG1:IgG2a ratio (Fig. 4C). Modification with CaP significantly increased the anti-BSA IgG1 and IgG2a titres (Fig. 4D and E) but decreased the anti-BSA IgG1:IgG2a ratio compared to soluble (0% CaP) PCMC formulations (Fig. 4F). The results above demonstrated that CaP modification had an adjuvant effect on PCMC-induced antigen responses in vivo, although increasing the CaP loading from 6 to 20% did not have a significantly consistent dose-dependent effect. To investigate this further, mice were immunised with a single dose of 0%, 6%, 12% or 20% CaP PCMCs loaded with 6 μg/dose each of DT and CyaA* and the kinetics of the serum antigen-specific IgG responses determined up to 84 d post-immunisation.

Location: All versions of the guidelines are available for downlo

Location: All versions of the guidelines are available for download at: http://guidance.nice.org.uk/CG124Description: C646 The full guideline is a large (664 pages) document reviewing the scientific evidence for the clinical and cost-effectiveness of different interventions to manage hip fracture in adults. The guideline begins with an outline of the scope and summary of methods used to review the evidence (Chapters 1–3), followed by a useful overview of the full guideline (Chapter 4). The main body of the guideline is divided into 9 chapters (Chapters 5–13) addressing a range of clinical questions such as imaging options, timing of surgery,

analgesia and surgical procedures. The main sections of interest to physiotherapists are Chapters 11 and 12 which review the evidence for mobilisation strategies (comparing early versus delayed mobilisation, and examining intensity of physiotherapy required) and multidisciplinary management after hip fracture in hospital and in the community. These chapters

are followed by 10 appendices which provide more details on the review protocols, literature search strategies, evidence tables and forest plots, and high priority research recommendations. “
“Latest update: April 2011. Next update: Not indicated. Patient group: Adults with osteoporosis-related health care problems. Intended audience: Physical therapists involved in the management of patients with osteoporosis. Additional

versions: The this website KNGF Guidelines for Physical Therapy in Patients with Osteoporosis consist of the main document and a flowchart, and replace a 2005 version. They are based on the osteoporosis guideline published by the Dutch College of General Practitioners and the multidisciplinary Dutch Guideline on Osteoporosis and Fracture Prevention (Osteoporose en Fractuurpreventie).Expert working group: A group of Dutch physical therapists compiled the guidelines, based on the recommendations in the Dutch Guideline on Osteoporosis and Fracture Prevention made by a multidisciplinary working party including medical specialists, physical therapists and other health professionals, under the auspices of the Dutch Institute for Healthcare Improvement. Funded by: Not indicated. Consultation with: An expert multidisciplinary Methisazone advisory group of 14, including consumer representatives contributed to this guideline. Approved by: The Royal Dutch Society for Physical Therapy (Koninklijk Nederlands Genootschap voor Fysiotherapie, KNGF). Location: The guidelines are available in English at: https://www.kngfrichtlijnen.nl/654/KNGF-Guidelines-in-English.htm Description: The guidelines consist of a 19-page document presenting recommendations for physical therapists regarding the assessment, diagnostic process and management of people with primary or secondary osteoporosis.

The topographies of these PCs show only a rough correspondence wi

The topographies of these PCs show only a rough correspondence with the outlines

of the FFA and PPA. For example, the first PC, whose tuning profile showed positive responses only for human faces, Selleckchem Stem Cell Compound Library has positive weights only in small subregions of the FFA. The fifth PC, whose tuning profile showed positive responses to both human and nonhuman animal faces, has positive weights in most, but not all, of the FFA, including the same subregions that had positive weights for the first PC, as well as in more posterior VT regions outside of the FFA. The second PC, which was associated with stronger responses to objects—especially houses—than faces, has only negative weights in the FFA and only positive weights in the PPA, but the topography of positive responses extends into a much larger region of medial VT cortex. By contrast, the third PC,

which also was associated with stronger responses to objects than faces but with a preference for small objects over houses, has a mixture Selleckchem PR-171 of positive and negative weights in both the FFA and PPA, with stronger positive weights in cortex between these regions and in the inferior temporal gyrus. Overall, these results show that the PCA-defined dimensions capture a functional topography in VT cortex that has more complexity and a finer spatial scale than that defined by large category-selective regions such as the FFA and PPA. The topographies for the PCs in the common model that best capture the variance in responses to the movie, a complex natural stimulus, did not correspond well with the category-selective Liothyronine Sodium regions, the FFA and PPA, that are identified based on responses to still images of a limited variety of stimuli. We next asked whether the category selectivity that defines these regions is preserved in the 35-dimensional

representational space of our model. First, we defined a dimension in the model space based on a linear discriminant that contrasts the mean response vector to faces and the mean response vector to houses and objects. The mean response vectors were based on group data in the face and object perception experiment. We then plotted the voxel weights for this dimension in the native anatomical spaces for individual subjects (Figure 6A; Figure S1F). Unlike the topographies for principal components, the voxel weights for this faces-versus-objects dimension have a topography that corresponds well with the boundaries of individually defined FFAs. Thus, when the response-tuning profiles are modeled with this single dimension, the face selectivity of FFA voxels is evident, but this dimension does not capture the fine-scale topography in the FFA that is the basis for decoding finer distinctions among faces or among nonface objects. By contrast, the dimensions in the common model do capture these distinctions.